Public opinionTerrorism pushes liberals to think more like conservatives

Published 21 December 2015

Liberals’ attitudes in the United Kingdom toward Muslims and immigrants became more like those of conservatives following the 7 July 2005 bombings in London, new research shows. Data from two nationally representative surveys of British citizens revealed that feelings of national loyalty increased and endorsement of equality decreased among political liberals following the terrorist attack. “Our findings show that terrorism shifts public attitudes towards greater loyalty to the in-group, less concern with fairness, and greater prejudice against Muslims and immigrants, but it seems that this effect is stronger on those who are politically left-leaning than those who are right-leaning,” explained one of the researchers.

British landmarks lit in the French tricolor // Source: commons.wikimedia.org

Liberals’ attitudes in the United Kingdom toward Muslims and immigrants became more like those of conservatives following the 7 July 2005 bombings in London, new research shows. Data from two nationally representative surveys of British citizens revealed that feelings of national loyalty increased and endorsement of equality decreased among political liberals following the terrorist attack. The findings are published in Psychological Science, a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

Terrorist attacks on major international capital cities such as Paris, Ankara, or London are rare and dramatic events that undoubtedly shape public and political opinion. But whose attitudes do they affect most, and in what way?

“Our findings show that terrorism shifts public attitudes towards greater loyalty to the in-group, less concern with fairness, and greater prejudice against Muslims and immigrants, but it seems that this effect is stronger on those who are politically left-leaning than those who are right-leaning,” explain psychological scientists from the Center for the Study of Group Processes at the University of Kent.

“The overall impact is to create a climate in which it may be harder to promote or sustain intergroup tolerance, inclusiveness and trust,” says Julie Van de Vyver of the University of Kent, one of the authors on the study.

APS reports that research from psychological science has shown that people often adopt ideological belief systems that reduce their feelings of threat. Based on these findings, the research team hypothesized that the bombings would cause liberals to shift moral perspectives in favor of protecting the in-group, akin to the values typically reported by political conservatives. They speculated that this shift would ultimately lead to an increase in prejudice toward the out-group among liberals.

Historic survey evidence gathered by two of the study authors, Diane Houston and Dominic Abrams, provided the research team with real-world insight. The researchers analyzed newly available data from two nationally representative surveys, administered about six weeks before and one month after the 7 July 2005 bombings in London. The bombings, which occurred on public transport, led to the deaths of fifty-two people and injury of 770 people. The bombings were part of an Al Qaeda attack carried out by three British-born Muslims from immigrant families and one Jamaican convert to Islam.

In the two surveys, participants rated their agreement with statements that represented four moral foundations: in-group loyalty (that is, “I feel loyal to Britain despite any faults it may have”), authority-respect (that is, “I think people should follow rules at all times, even when no one is watching”), harm-care (that is, “I want everyone to be treated justly, even people I do not know. It is important to me to protect the weak in society), and fairness-reciprocity (that is, “There should be equality for all groups in Britain”).

Participants also rated their agreement with statements about attitudes toward Muslims (for example, “Britain would lose its identity if more Muslims came to live in Britain”) and immigrants (for example, “Government spends too much money assisting immigrants”).

As expected, attitudes towards Muslims and toward immigrants were more negative following the attacks than before, but only among liberals; conservatives’ views stayed relatively constant. Thus, liberals’ attitudes seemed to shift toward those of conservatives following the bombings.

This increased prejudice was accounted for by changes in liberals’ moral foundations. Specifically, liberals showed an increase in in-group loyalty and a decrease in fairness, and these shifts accounted for their negative attitudes toward Muslims and immigrants.

The results show that people’s moral perspectives aren’t necessarily constant — they can change according to the immediate context.

“An important challenge following dramatic terrorist attacks is to know how to engage with public perceptions and attitudes, for example to prevent an upsurge in prejudice and its effects,” says Abrams.

“For people working to tackle prejudice, it is important to be aware that terror events may have different effects on the attitudes of people who start from different political orientations,” the researchers write.

Based on these findings, the researchers argue that terrorist attacks may ultimately lead conservatives to consolidate their existing priorities, making them resistant to change; at the same time, such attacks may prompt a shift in liberals’ priorities toward more prejudiced attitudes.

This shift in attitudes may be reflected in the U.K. parliament’s recent decision, following the November attacks in Paris, to approve bombing missions in Syria — a reversal of its decision in 2013. The researchers note that the greatest change in voting occurred among Labor Members of Parliament, who fall on the left end of the political spectrum; they showed a 20 percent increase in support for the bombing missions from 2013 to 2015.

— Read more in Julie Van de Vyver et al., “Boosting Belligerence: How the July 7, 2005, London Bombings Affected Liberals’ Moral Foundations and Prejudice,” Psychological Science (16 December 2015) (doi: 10.1177/0956797615615584)